Short Communication

Women Can’t Have It All: Benevolent Sexism Predicts Attitudes Toward Working (vs. Stay-at-Home) Mothers

Marta Szastok*a, Małgorzata Kossowskaa, Joanna Pyrkosz-Pacynab

Social Psychological Bulletin, 2019, Vol. 14(1), Article e29461, https://doi.org/10.32872/spb.v14i1.29461

Received: 2018-09-11. Accepted: 2019-03-15. Published (VoR): 2019-05-15.

Handling Editor: Pascal Gygax, University of Fribourg, Fribourg, Switzerland

*Corresponding author at: Institute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University, Ingardena 6, 30-060 Kraków, Poland. E-mail: marta.szastok@gmail.com

This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Abstract

The aim of the present paper was to test differences in perceptions towards a woman who took a 3-month maternity leave (a working mother) as opposed to a 3-year maternity leave (a stay-at-home mother), and then to apply the ambivalent sexism theory to predict those differences. We expected that in Poland, where motherhood is highly appreciated, it is especially benevolent (not hostile) sexism that predicts less positive attitudes toward working mothers, compared to stay-at-home mothers. In two studies, we found that the working mother was perceived as less warm, less effective as a parent and less interpersonally appealing and more successful at work. Additionally, although the stay-at-home mother was evaluated as less successful at work, she was not perceived as less competent. We discuss this as a reflection of the “Mother-Pole” phenomenon, where mothers in Poland are perceived as not only kind, but also competent. Afterward, we showed that benevolent (but not hostile) sexism predicts differences in perceiving the stay-at-home mother and working mother. Participants higher in benevolent sexism rated the stay-at-home mother as warmer, more parenting-effective and more interpersonally appealing compared to the working mother, while participants lower in benevolent sexism perceived them equally well. Studies suggest that benevolent sexism predicts a more positive perception of traditional mothers (as opposed to nontraditional mothers), and at the same time, maintains the status quo of traditional gender relations.

Keywords: benevolent sexism, motherhood, nontraditional women, working mothers

Studies consistently reveal that the traditional worldview about women’s motherhood-related obligations is still valid (e.g., CBOS, 2013). There is a preference for the traditional division of labor, where men are breadwinners and women are homemakers. Although acceptance of working mothers is growing, the majority agrees that when a child is aged below three, the woman should be a stay-at-home mother (Antosz, 2012). Thus, working mothers still violate traditional gender norms (Brescoll & Uhlmann, 2005).

Because of motherhood-related responsibilities, women drop out of the job market, earn less (Sigle-Rushton & Waldfogel, 2007) and are less likely to be hired or promoted (Cuddy, Fiske, & Glick, 2004). The motherhood penalty (Correll, Benard, & Paik, 2007) affects women’s careers and reinforces gender inequality in the labor market – especially in Poland, where the index of employment among mothers is one of the lowest in Europe (Kurowska, 2010). Additionally, women who combine career with motherhood are perceived as interpersonally inadequate (e.g., Brescoll & Uhlmann, 2005).

The aim of this paper was to test differences in the perception of career woman who took a 3-month maternity leave (working mother) as opposed to a 3-year maternity leave (stay-at-home mother), and then to apply the ambivalent sexism theory (Glick & Fiske, 1996) to predict those differences. The effect of benevolent and hostile sexism on attitudes toward traditional/nontraditional mothers has been tested before (e.g., Gaunt, 2013). Results suggest that individuals higher in benevolent and hostile sexism accept women who comply with traditional gender roles more. The replication of these results in the Polish context, where mothers are highly appreciated and idealized (Hryciuk & Korolczuk, 2012) merits investigation. As negative attitudes toward women who want to combine a successful career with motherhood do foster gender inequality, studies that define predictors of those attitudes also seem important. In fact, we expected that mothers in Poland might not evoke hostility, and, because of that, benevolent sexism would especially moderate attitudes toward stay-at-home/working mothers.

Perception of Stay-at-Home vs. Working Mothers

According to the stereotype content model (e.g., Cuddy, Fiske, & Glick, 2008), each group can be characterized by two dimensions: warmth and competence. Women are perceived as warmer, and less competent than men. However, this is mostly true for women who meet expectations arising from traditional norms (e.g., homemakers; Eagly & Mladinic, 1994). Women who violate the prescriptive aspect of gender stereotypes evoke negative reactions and experience a backlash (e.g., Rudman, 1998). Compared to traditional women, nontraditional women (e.g., feminists) are more respected for their competence. However, at the same time, they are perceived as colder and interpersonally inadequate. A unique case of a woman who occupies a nontraditional gender role is a working mother. Indeed, previous research has shown that although working mothers (compared to stay-at-home mothers) are perceived as more competent and job-successful, they are also evaluated as colder, less devoted to motherhood, and less likeable (Brescoll & Uhlmann, 2005; Bridges, Etaugh, & Barnes-Farrell, 2002; Coleman & Franiuk, 2011; Cuddy et al., 2004; Okimoto & Heilman, 2012). In line with previous studies, we expected that while a working mother would be appreciated for her professional skills, she would be perceived as colder and less parenting-effective than a stay-at-home mother. Also, as nontraditional women evoke a backlash that could manifest in social isolation (Rudman & Fairchild, 2004), we expected a working mother to be evaluated as less interpersonally appealing. However, contrary to the stereotype content model, but considering the Polish context, we did not expect she would be perceived as more competent.

One of the essential elements of Poland’s national identity and culture is the “Mother-Pole” phenomenon, in which mothers are warm and kind, and, at the same time, competent, multitasking, and strong (Hryciuk & Korolczuk, 2012). This phenomenon is rooted in the 19th century, the time of the Partitions. While men “fought on the battlefield”, women, left alone, had to raise children with a strong national identity (Frąckowiak-Sochańska, 2011) and fulfill stereotypically male obligations. In 20th century Communist Poland, women were also expected to efficiently combine domestic and professional duties (Fidelis, 2004; Gal & Kligman, 2000; Krause, 2016; Malinowska, 1995; Titkow, 2001). In line with the social role theory (Eagly, 1987), it might have shaped the perception of mothers as warm and competent. Moreover, women in Poland are perceived as having an inner strength (resourcefulness) that helps them to cope with the adversities of life (Mikołajczak, 2016). We expected the stay-at-home (vs. working) mother to be perceived as equally competent.

Sexism and Attitudes Toward Working Mothers

According to the ambivalent sexism theory (Glick & Fiske, 1996), sexism is a multidimensional construct that encompasses hostile and benevolent attitudes toward women. While hostile sexism is evident antipathy toward women (Allport, 1954), benevolent sexism is seemingly positive and flattering (e.g., Barreto & Ellemers, 2005; Bohner, Ahlborn, & Steiner, 2010), yet actually a patronizing belief, according to which women are pure, moral but incompetent. Although these concepts differ in tone, they work together like the proverbial carrot and stick (Jackman, 1994) to justify the traditional system of gender relations, where women are caregivers and men are breadwinners (Eagly, 1987; Glick et al., 2000, 2004; Jost, Banaji, & Nosek, 2004). Several studies have shown that benevolent sexism is related to positive evaluations of women who comply with traditional gender norms, and hostile sexism predicts negative evaluations of women who violate them (e.g., Becker, 2010; Gaunt, 2013; Glick, Diebold, Bailey-Werner, & Zhu, 1997; Hebl, King, Glick, Singletary, & Kazama, 2007; Kofta, Soral, Kwiatkowska, Kapusta, & Mikołajczak, 2016). For example, Glick et al. (1997) found that hostile sexism predicted negative attitudes toward career women and benevolent sexism predicted positive attitudes toward homemakers. However, benevolent sexism is also related to negative attitudes toward female norm violators, whose behavior is perceived as inappropriate (e.g., Abrams, Viki, Masser, & Bohner, 2003; Viki & Abrams, 2002).

In line with previous studies, we could expect that both benevolent and hostile sexism would predict less positive attitudes toward working mothers. However, Poland has a firm Catholic identity. In terms of gender roles, this implies a strong identification of women as mothers and caregivers (Janion, 1996, 2006; Łaciak, 2012). As mothers in Poland are perceived highly favorably, it is possible they will not raise hostility. Also, due to its assumptions that women are predisposed to be mothers, we expected benevolent sexism to be a more robust predictor of attitudes toward mothers, even those who combine motherhood with professional development.

Research Overview

To test our hypothesis, we conducted two studies. In the pilot study we tested whether the duration of the maternity leave taken predicts the way a mother is perceived. Participants were given a scenario (prepared specifically for this study) about a successful woman who had recently become a mother. We manipulated the length of the maternity leave: three years in a stay-at-home mother condition versus three months in a working mother condition.i After reading the scenario, participants evaluated the warmth, competence, parenting and job effectiveness of the female character. We expected the stay-at-home mother to be perceived as warmer, equally competent (“Mother-Pole” phenomenon), more parenting-effective and less successful at work compared to the working mother. Additionally, as violating traditional norms may have an implication for liking and social ostracism (desire to interact), we measured the general impression the female character evoked (pilot study) and her interpersonal appeal (main study).

In the main study, we added benevolent and hostile sexism into the model to investigate whether beliefs moderate the effect of the mother’s role. We expected that benevolent sexism would especially moderate attitudes toward stay-at-home/working mothers. We hypothesized that participants higher in benevolent sexism would evaluate the stay-at-home mother as warmer, more parenting effective and more interpersonally appealing than the working mother, while participants lower in benevolent sexism would evaluate them equally well. As benevolent sexism is associated with the stereotype that women are incompetent (Glick & Fiske, 1996), we expected participants higher in sexism to perceive the stay-at-home mother as less competent and successful than the working mother, while participants lower in sexism would evaluate them similarly.

Pilot Study

Method

Participants

A group of 90 students (51 women, Mage = 21.71, SD = 2.21) attending Language Studies voluntarily participated in the study during classes.

Materials and Procedure

Perception of the stay-at-home/working mother

Participants read a short scenario that described a new mother. Depending on the experimental condition, she took a 3-year (stay-at-home mother) or 3-month maternity leave (working mother). The full scenario was as follows: “Agnes is 34. She and her husband have recently become parents. Before that, she was employed as a project manager in a trade organization. She would leave for work at 8 a.m. and come back home at 6 p.m. Sometimes, she had to work overtime or go for business trips that were a few days long. After the baby was born, she took 3-year/3-month break from work”. After reading the scenario, the participants evaluated Agnes using the following measures. If not mentioned otherwise, dependent variables were measured on a 1 (definitely not) to 7 (definitely yes) scale.

Warmth and competence

Participants indicated to what extent the female character was warm, friendly, helpful, moral, honest, good (warmth, α = .94) and competent, intelligent and talented (competence, α = .91; Fiske, Cuddy, & Glick, 2007).

Parenting effectiveness

We measured the perceived parenting effectiveness of the character with three statements: “She is a good parent”; “She will have a good relationship with her child”; “She and her child will have a strong bond” (α = .95; Coleman & Franiuk, 2011).

Job effectiveness

The perceived job effectiveness of the character was measured with three statements: “She is a good employee”, “She will succeed in her career”, “She will be appreciated by her superiors and colleagues” (α = .88; Coleman & Franiuk, 2011).

General impression

To capture the general impression of the character, participants were asked to indicate their general impression of Agnes using a 1 (definitely unfavorable) to 7 (definitely favorable) scale.

Results

To capture the effect of role (stay-at-home/working mother) on the perception of the scenario’s female character, we conducted a multivariate analysis of variation (MANOVA)ii. Compared to the working mother, the stay-at-home mother was perceived as warmer, F(1, 88) = 25.67, p < .001, η2 = .226, and equally competent, F(1, 88) = 0.01, p = .914, η2 = .000. She was also perceived as more parenting-effective, F(1, 88) = 126.34, p < .001, η2 = .589, and less job-effective, F(1, 88) = 52.61, p < .001, η2 = .374). Finally, participants had a more favorable impression of her, F(1, 88) = 27.72, p < .001, η2 = .240. Descriptive statistics and correlations between variables are presented in Table 1.

Table 1

Correlations Between Variables and Descriptive Statistics (Pilot Study)

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 Stay-at-home mother
Working mother
M 95% CI M 95% CI
1. Warmth - .61*** .65*** .23 .66*** 5.48 [5.18, 5.79] 4.28 [3.91, 4.64]
2. Competence .65*** - .37* .35* .40* 5.59 [5.31, 5.87] 6.62 [5.25, 5.98]
3. Parenting effectiveness .70*** .56*** - -.11 .69*** 6.26 [5.99, 6.52] 3.48 [3.06, 3.89]
4. Job effectiveness .21 .38* .26 - .08 4.39 [4.01, 4.76] 6.03 [5.76, 6.30]
5. General impression .57*** .45** .66*** .43** - 5.80 [5.40, 6.19] 4.35 [3.96, 4.74]

Note. Correlations for the stay-at-home mother (n = 44) are presented below the diagonal, and correlations for the working mother (n = 46) are presented above the diagonal.

*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

Discussion

As expected the stay-at-home mother (as opposed to the working mother) was perceived as warmer and more parenting-effective. Additionally, in line with the “Mother-Pole” phenomenon, but contrary to the stereotype content model, the stay-at-home mother was not evaluated as less competent. The fact that the traditional woman was marked high on both warmth and competence is consistent with results of other research conducted in Poland (e.g., Mikołajczak, Pietrzak, & Winiewski, 2009; Pietrzak, Mikołajczak, Chroł, & Markiewicz, 2011). Still, the stay-at-home mother was perceived as less job-effective than the working mother.

In the main study, we aimed to test whether benevolent and hostile sexism moderate attitudes toward stay-at-home/working mothers. Given the Polish context, we expected that benevolent sexism would be a more robust predictor of attitudes toward mothers. We predicted that participants high (vs. low) in benevolent sexism would accept the stay-at-home (vs. working) mother more. Instead of students, we recruited a more diverse sample through an online research panel, but we did not expect that this would affect the results. We also replaced the one-item measurement of overall impression with four items measuring interpersonal appeal.

Main Study

Method

Participants

A group of 101 participants (56 women, Mage = 32.88, SD = 12.07) was recruited via the researchonline.pl research portal. Each participant was paid 3 PLN (about 1$) for participating.

Materials and Procedure

Sexism

We measured benevolent (e.g., “Many women have a quality of purity that few men possess”; α = .88, M = 3.74; 95% CI [3.53, 3.94]) and hostile sexism (e.g., “Women are too easily offended”; α = .86, M = 3.95; 95% CI [3.77, 4.13]) with the Ambivalent Sexism Scale (ASI, Glick & Fiske, 1996; Polish adaptation by Mikołajczak & Pietrzak, 2014). Participants indicated their agreement with statements on a 0 (I strongly disagree) to 5 (I strongly agree) scale. Scales were positively correlated, r(101) = .46, p < .001.

Perception of the stay-at-home/working mother

To measure attitudes towards the stay-at-home/working mother, we used the same scenarios as in the pilot study. Participants first read the scenario and then evaluated the female character using the measures below. Dependent variables were measured on 1 (definitely not) to 7 (definitely yes) scales.

Warmth, competence, parenting effectiveness and job effectiveness

To measure the perceived warmth (α = .95), competence (α = .88), parenting effectiveness (α = .98) and job effectiveness (α = .92) of the female character, we used the same scales as in the pilot study.

Interpersonal appeal

To show their desire to interact with the female character, participants indicated their agreement with four statements: “Would you like her to be your neighbor?”, “Would you like her to be your friend?”, “Would you like your children to be friends with hers?”, “Would you like to work with her?” (α = .92; Okimoto & Heilman, 2012).

Results

We conducted a MANOVA to capture the effect of role (stay-at-home/working mother) on the perception of the female character. Afterwards, we included benevolent and hostile sexism in the model and conducted a series of multiple moderation analyses with PROCESS (Model 2; Hayes, 2013). For every dependent variable, we tested the model in which the woman’s role served as an independent variable, while benevolent and hostile sexism were the moderators. However, as the two-way interaction of role and hostile sexism was never significant (all ps > .285), we eventually tested the simple moderation model, with benevolent sexism as a single moderator and hostile sexism as a control variable (Model 1)iii. We coded the stay-at-home mother as 0 and the working mother as 1. The main effect of benevolent sexism on the dependent variables was never significant (all ps > .224). For the faculty of presentation, we will describe the results separately for every dependent variable. Descriptive statistics and correlations between variables are presented in Table 2.

Table 2

Correlations Between Variables and Descriptive Statistics (Main Study)

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Stay-at-home mother
Working mother
M 95% CI M 95% CI
1. Warmth - .33* .76*** .25 .68*** -.25 .05 5.34 [5.02, 5.67] 4.30 [3.98, 4.62]
2. Competence .70*** - .10 .57*** .23 -.02 -.05 5.55 [5.25, 5.85] 5.27 [4.98, 5.55]
3. Parenting effectiveness .78*** .57*** - .06 .67*** -.10 .17 5.91 [5.60, 6.22] 3.79 [3.37, 4.21]
4. Job effectiveness .29 .37** .33* - .20 .08 .17 4.96 [4.56, 5.37] 5.66 [5.39, 5.93]
5. Interpersonal appeal .71*** .62*** .69* .55*** - -.13 .23 5.63 [5.26, 5.99] 4.43 [4.09, 4.77]
6. Benevolent sexism .33* .15 .41** .13 .37* - .47***
7. Hostile sexism .33* .11 .21 .20 .31* .44** -

Note. Correlations for the stay-at-home mother (n = 46) are presented below the diagonal, and correlations for the working mother (n = 50) are presented above the diagonal.

*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

Warmth

The stay-at-home mother was perceived as warmer than the working mother, F(1, 94) = 21.47, p < .001, η2 = .186. A two-way interaction of woman’s role and benevolent sexism (BS) was significant, b = -0.63, SE = 0.13, p = .003; 95% CI [-1.04, -0.22], R2 change = .073. Closer inspections of simple slopes showed participants low in BS (-1 SD) perceived the stay-at-home and working mom as equally warm, b = -0.34, SE = 0.31, β = -.14, p = .275, 95% CI [-0.96, 0.28]; Mstay-at-home = 5.01, Mworking = 4.67, while participants high in BS (+1 SD) evaluated the stay-at-home mother as warmer, b = -1.69, SE = 0.31, β = -.70, p < .001, 95% CI [-2.30, -1.08]; Mstay-at-home = 5.57, Mworking = 3.88. This is presented in Figure 1.

Click to enlarge
spb.v14i1.29461-f1
Figure 1

The two-way interaction of role (stay-at-home mother vs. working mother) and benevolent sexism on perceived warmth of the female character (main study).

Competence

Neither the main effect of role, F(1, 94) = 1.93, p = .168, η2 = .02, on assigned competence, nor the interaction with benevolent sexism was significant (p = .415).

Parenting Effectiveness

The stay-at-home mother was perceived as a better parent, F(1, 94) = 64.56, p < .001, η2 = .407. The interaction of role and benevolent sexism was significant, b = -0.60, SE = 0.24, p = .017, 95% CI [-1.08, -0.11], R2 change = .035. A simple slope analysis revealed that while participants low in BS, -1 SD; b = -1.43, SE = 0.37, β = -.43, p < .001, 95% CI [-2.16, -0.69]; Mstay-at-home = 5.49, Mworking = 4.07, and high in BS, +1 SD; b = -2.70, SE = 0.36, β = -.81, p < .001, 95% CI [-3.42, -1.98]; Mstay-at-home = 6.20, Mworking = 3.50, both perceived the stay-at-home mother as more parenting-effective, this effect was stronger among those high in BS.

Job-Effectiveness

The stay-at-home mother was evaluated as a worse employee, F(1, 94) = 8.55, p = .004, η2 = .083. The two-way interaction of role and benevolent sexism was not significant (p = .559).

Interpersonal Appeal

The stay-at-home mother was perceived as more interpersonally appealing, F(1, 94) = 23.33, p < .001, η2 = .199. The main effect of role was qualified by a significant interaction with benevolent sexism, b = -0.63, SE = 0.22, p = .006; 95% CI [-1.07, -1.19], R2 change = .06. While participants low in BS (-1 SD) perceived the women in both conditions similarly, b = -0.46, SE = 0.34, β = -.17, p = .179, 95% CI [-1.13, 0.21]; Mstay-at-home = 5.21, Mworking = 4.75, participants high in BS (+1 SD) perceived the working mother as less appealing, b = -1.81, SE = 0.33, β = -.67, p < .001, 95% CI [-2.46, -1.15]; Mstay-at-home = 5.90, Mworking = 4.10.

Discussion

As expected, benevolent sexism moderated attitudes toward the stay-at-home/working mother. Participants higher in benevolent sexism perceived the working mother as less warm and interpersonally appealing, while participants lower in benevolent sexism evaluated them equally well. Although both groups of participants evaluated the working mothers as less parenting-effective, the effect was stronger among participants high in benevolent sexism. Results suggest that benevolent sexism predicts a more favorable perception of the stay-at-home mother. However, the effect of benevolent sexism was significant only on warmth-related dimensions; assigned competence and job effectiveness was not predicted. Hostile sexism did not moderate the effect of role. We will discuss these results in the general discussion.

General Discussion

The main goal of this paper was to test whether benevolent and hostile sexism predicts differences in perceiving stay-at-home/working mothers. As sexism justifies traditional gender relations, we hypothesized that participants higher in benevolent sexism would perceive the working mother less favorably than the stay-at-home mother, as the former violates traditional gender norms. At the same time, we expected participants low in benevolent sexism to evaluate the working and stay-at-home mothers equally well. Given that mothers are greatly appreciated in Poland, we did not expect hostile sexism to moderate attitudes.

In two studies, we consistently showed that the working mother was perceived less positively than the stay-at-home mother and experienced a backlash. She was evaluated as less warm, less parenting-effective and less interpersonally appealing. Any attempt to combine motherhood with a professional career leads to disapproval and social isolation.

As expected, the stay-at-home mother was not perceived as less competent. Although one possible explanation is that the contents of the scenario emphasized the competence of the female character, we interpreted this as a reflection of the “Mother Pole” phenomenon, in which mothers in Poland are perceived as both kind and skillful. This result suggests that the prediction of the stereotype content model cannot be applied universally, and that prevailing representations may affect how certain subtypes of women are perceived. Although the stay-at-home mother was not evaluated as less intelligent or talented, the comparison revealed significant differences in perceived job effectiveness. The mother who took a three-year maternity leave was evaluated as a less effective employee. This is in line with previous studies that have shown that mothers in the job market hit the maternal wall and are discriminated against (e.g., Anderson, Binder, & Krause, 2003; Crosby, Williams, & Biernat, 2004). Gender inequality in the division of household duties (Bobrowicz, 2007; CBOS, 2018; Fuwa & Cohen, 2007) or short supply of daycare (GUS, 2016) often makes a longer maternity leave inevitable. The negative perception of professional skills of mothers who spend more time on leave seems to have a substantial impact on the persistence of gender inequality in the labor market.

As expected, we have found that benevolent sexism predicts differences in perceiving stay-at-home/working mothers. Participants low in benevolent sexism evaluated both as equally warm and interpersonally appealing, while those high in benevolent sexism ranked the stay-at-home mother higher. Benevolent sexism predicted stereotype-driven evaluations and fostered a backlash against the working mother. Although the interaction of role and benevolent sexism with respect to parenting effectiveness was significant, both participants low and high in benevolent sexism evaluated the working mother as less parenting-effective (still, the effect was stronger among those high in benevolent sexism). It is likely that even those who do not endorse benevolent sexism hold a belief that women cannot work and be a good mother at the same time. Nevertheless, we managed to replicate the results known from previous research (e.g., Abrams et al., 2003): that benevolent sexism is related to less positive attitudes toward women who violate traditional gender norms.

Contrary to previous research (e.g., Hebl et al., 2007), hostile sexism did not predict the perception of the stay-at-home/working mother. Though this may seem surprising (as, theoretically, hostile sexism is related to negative evaluations of traditional norm violators; Glick & Fiske, 1996), there are several possible explanations. Firstly, it is benevolent (not hostile) sexism that involves the assumption that due to their moral superiority women are prepared to be mothers and this is the role they should play in society. Secondly, the nontraditional woman from the scenario might not have been nontraditional enough to raise hostility – after all, she was a mother (Szastok, Kossowska, & de Lemus, 2019). It may be due to the Polish idealization of mothers that hostile beliefs did not moderate those attitudes. Finally, some studies have already shown that benevolent sexism is a more powerful predictor of attitudes that justify traditional gender norms (e.g., Abrams et al., 2003; Becker & Wright, 2011; Jost & Kay, 2005). Further research is needed to distinguish the specific effect of benevolent and hostile sexism.

As sexism is related to the assumption that women are incompetent, we expected participants higher in sexism to evaluate the stay-at-home mother as less competent and successful at work. However, neither hostile nor benevolent sexism interacted with the woman’s role on competence and job effectiveness. Since perceived competence is a less critical dimension in female stereotypes (e.g., Prentice & Carranza, 2002), it may be that it is less sensitive to the effect of prejudice, as it matters less for the overall impression.

In these studies, participants read a scenario describing a career woman who became a mother. The contents in the stay-at-home mother scenario do not match the usual definition of a stay-at-home mother, who, above all, is a caregiver (e.g., Gaunt, 2013). However, from the perspective of gender equality, mothers have to participate in the job market. The stay-at-home mother scenario (only) involved a 3-year maternity leave, as that seems useful to measure attitudes toward women who combine motherhood with a career (giving them different time priorities). This way, we referred more to the real-life experiences of women. Additionally, as the experimental conditions differed only in the duration of the leave, the interpretation of the result was more straightforward.

The studies found that, in Poland, it is benevolent sexism that moderates attitudes toward the stay-at-home/working mother. However, these results must be interpreted with caution. Future research may define the role of benevolent and hostile sexism in shaping attitudes toward mothers in different cultural contexts while, at the same, time providing cumulative evidence for these effects (see Cumming & Calin-Jageman, 2017, for a discussion on the need for cumulative evidence).

The results confirm that women are between a rock and a hard place. If they show a strong commitment to their professional career, they will inevitably be perceived as interpersonally inadequate. In turn, if they devote more time to childcare, it will cost them professional credibility. Our findings suggest that women cannot have it all: a professional career does not go hand in hand with motherhood. This is especially true among those high in benevolent sexism, as they present more positive attitudes toward mothers who take longer maternity leaves. By contrast, the results suggest that participants lower in benevolent sexism did not blame mothers (or blamed them less) for combining a career with motherhood. To conclude, we showed that benevolent sexism predicts a more positive perception of women in the traditional role and, therefore, maintains the traditional gender relations.

Notes

i) Mothers in Poland are entitled to 1 year of paid and 3 years of unpaid maternity leave.

ii) We controlled all statistical models described in this paper for participants’ gender. As it was not a significant factor, we will not describe the effect of gender in more detail.

iii) Regardless of whether or not we added the second dimension of sexism to the model as a covariate variable, we obtained similar results.

Funding

This research was supported by a grant from the National Science Centre (2011/03/N/HS6/02312).

Competing Interests

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Acknowledgments

The authors have no support to report.

Data Availability

For both studies a dataset is freely available (see the Supplementary Materials section).

Supplementary Materials

The following Supplementary Materials are available via the PsychArchives repository (for access see Index of Supplementary Materials below):

  1. Pilot Study Dataset

  2. Main Study Dataset

Index of Supplementary Materials

  • Szastok, M., Kossowska, M., & Pyrkosz-Pacyna, J. (2019). Supplementary materials to "Women can’t have it all: Benevolent sexism predicts attitudes toward working (vs. stay-at-home) mothers".PsychOpen. https://doi.org/10.23668/psycharchives.2431

References

  • Abrams, D., Viki, G. T., Masser, B., & Bohner, G. (2003). Perceptions of stranger and acquaintance rape: The role of benevolent and hostile sexism in victim blame and rape proclivity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(1), 111-125. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.1.111

  • Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Reading, MA, USA: Addison-Wesley.

  • Anderson, D. J., Binder, M., & Krause, K. (2003). The motherhood wage penalty revisited: Experience, heterogeneity, work effort, and work-schedule flexibility. Industrial & Labor Relations Review, 56(2), 273-294. https://doi.org/10.1177/001979390305600204

  • Antosz, P. (2012). Równe traktowanie standardem dobrego rządzenia. Raport z badań sondażowych [Equality as a good governance standard: The survey report]. Retrieved from http://www.spoleczenstwoobywatelskie.gov.pl/sites/default/ files/rowne_traktowanie_standardem_dobrego_rzadzenia__raport_z_badan_ ilosiowych_ost_0_0 .pdf

  • Barreto, M., & Ellemers, N. (2005). The burden of benevolent sexism: How it contributes to the maintenance of gender inequalities. European Journal of Social Psychology, 35(5), 633-642. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.270

  • Becker, J. C. (2010). Why do women endorse hostile and benevolent sexism? The role of salient female subtypes and personalization of sexist contents. Sex Roles, 62(7-8), 453-467. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-009-9707-4

  • Becker, J. C., & Wright, S. C. (2011). Yet another dark side of chivalry: Benevolent sexism undermines and hostile sexism motivates collective action for social change. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101(1), 62-77. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022615

  • Bobrowicz, B. (2007). Alokacja czasu: praca zawodowa i edukacja versus funkcje opiekuńcze i prace domowe [Time allocation: Professional work and education versus caretaking and housework]. In I. Kotowska, U. Sztanderska, & I. Wóycicka (Ed.), Aktywność zawodowa i edukacyjna a obowiązki rodzinne w Polsce: w świetle badan empirycznych [Professional and educational activity and family responsibilities in Poland: Empirical research]. Warszawa, Poland: Scholar.

  • Bohner, G., Ahlborn, K., & Steiner, R. (2010). How sexy are sexist men? Women’s perception of male response profiles in the ambivalent sexism inventory. Sex Roles, 62(7), 568-582. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-009-9665-x

  • Brescoll, V. L., & Uhlmann, E. L. (2005). Attitudes toward traditional and nontraditional parents. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 29(4), 436-445. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6402.2005.00244.x

  • Bridges, J. S., Etaugh, C., & Barnes-Farrell, J. (2002). Trait judgments of stay-at-home and employed parents: A function of social role and/or shifting standards? Psychology of Women Quarterly, 26(2), 140-150. https://doi.org/10.1111/1471-6402.00052

  • CBOS. (2013). O roli kobiet w rodzinie [On the women’s role in the family]. Retrieved from http://www.cbos.pl/SPISKOM.POL/2013/K_030_13.PDF

  • CBOS. (2018). Kobiety i mężczyźni w domu [Women and men at home]. Retrieved from https://www.cbos.pl/SPISKOM.POL/2018/K_127_18.PDF

  • Coleman, J. M., & Franiuk, R. (2011). Perceptions of mothers and fathers who take temporary work leave. Sex Roles, 64(5-6), 311-323. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-010-9918-8

  • Correll, S. J., Benard, B., & Paik, I. (2007). Getting a job: Is there a motherhood penalty? American Journal of Sociology, 112(5), 1297-1339. https://doi.org/10.1086/511799

  • Crosby, F. J., Williams, J. C., & Biernat, M. (2004). The maternal wall. The Journal of Social Issues, 60(4), 675-682. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0022-4537.2004.00379.x

  • Cuddy, A. J. C., Fiske, S. T., & Glick, P. (2004). When professionals become mothers, warmth doesn’t cut the ice. The Journal of Social Issues, 60(4), 701-718. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0022-4537.2004.00381.x

  • Cuddy, A. J. C., Fiske, S. T., & Glick, P. (2008). Warmth and competence as universal dimensions of social perception: The stereotype content model and the BIAS map. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 61-149. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(07)00002-0

  • Cumming, G., & Calin-Jageman, R. (2017). Introduction to the new statistics: Estimation, open science, and beyond. New York, NY, USA: Routledge.

  • Eagly, A. H. (1987). Sex differences in social behavior: A social-role interpretation. Hillsdale, NJ, USA: Erlbaum.

  • Eagly, A. H., & Mladinic, A. (1994). Are people prejudiced against women? Some answers from research on attitudes, gender stereotypes, and judgments of competence. European Review of Social Psychology, 5(1), 1-35. https://doi.org/10.1080/14792779543000002

  • Fidelis, M. (2004). Equality through protection: The politics of women’s employment in postwar Poland, 1945-1956. Slavic Review, 63(2), 301-324. https://doi.org/10.2307/3185730

  • Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., & Glick, P. (2007). Universal dimensions of social cognition: Warmth and competence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(2), 77-83. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.005

  • Frąckowiak-Sochańska, M. (2011). Postawy polskich kobiet wobec feminizmu. O samoograniczającej się świadomości feministycznej Polek [Attitudes of Polish women toward feminism: On the self-limiting feminist consciousness of Polish women]. In E. Malinowska (Ed.), Folia Sociologica, 39, 149-170.

  • Fuwa, M., & Cohen, P. N. (2007). Housework and social policy. Social Science Research, 36(2), 512-530. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssresearch.2006.04.005

  • Gal, S., & Kligman, G. (2000). The politics of gender after socialism: A comparative-historical essay. Princeton, NJ, USA: Princeton University Press.

  • Gaunt, R. (2013). Ambivalent sexism and perceptions of men and women who violate gendered family roles. Community Work & Family, 16(4), 401-416. https://doi.org/10.1080/13668803.2013.779231

  • Glick, P., Diebold, J., Bailey-Werner, B., & Zhu, L. (1997). The two faces of Adam: Ambivalent sexism and polarized attitudes toward women. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23(12), 1323-1334. https://doi.org/10.1177/01461672972312009

  • Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (1996). The Ambivalent Sexism Inventory: Differentiating hostile and benevolent sexism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(3), 491-512. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.70.3.491

  • Glick, P., Fiske, S. T., Mladinic, A., Saiz, J. L., Abrams, D., Masser, B., . . . López, W., (2000). Beyond prejudice as simple antipathy: Hostile and benevolent sexism across cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(5), 763-775. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.5.763

  • Glick, P., Lameiras, M., Fiske, S. T., Eckes, T., Masser, B., Volpato, C., . . . Wells, R., (2004). Bad but bold: Ambivalent attitudes toward men predict gender inequality in 16 nations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86(5), 713-728. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.86.5.713

  • GUS. (2016). Żłobki i kluby dziecięce w 2016 r. [Daycare and nurseries in 2016] Retrieved from https://stat.gov.pl/obszary-tematyczne/dzieci-i-rodzina/dzieci/zlobki-i-kluby-dzieciece-w-2016-r-,3,4.html

  • Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York, NY, USA: The Guilford Press.

  • Hebl, M. R., King, E. B., Glick, P., Singletary, S., & Kazama, S. (2007). Hostile and benevolent reactions toward pregnant women: Complementary interpersonal punishments and rewards that maintain traditional roles. The Journal of Applied Psychology, 92(6), 1499-1511. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.92.6.1499

  • Hryciuk, R. E., & Korolczuk, E. (2012). Pożegnanie z Matką Polką? [Farewell to the Mother Pole?]. Warsaw, Poland: Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Warszawskiego.

  • Jackman, M. R. (1994). The velvet glove: Paternalism and conflict in gender, class, and race relations. Berkeley, CA, USA: University of California Press.

  • Janion, M. (1996). Kobiety i duch inności [Women and the spirit of otherness]. Warsaw, Poland: Sic.

  • Janion, M. (2006). Niesamowita Słowiańszczyzna [The incredible Slavic world]. Cracow, Poland: Wydawnictwo Literackie.

  • Jost, J. T., Banaji, M. R., & Nosek, B. A. (2004). A decade of system justification theory: Accumulated evidence of conscious and unconscious bolstering of the status-quo. Political Psychology, 25(6), 881-919. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2004.00402.x

  • Jost, J. T., & Kay, A. C. (2005). Exposure to benevolent sexism and complementary gender stereotypes: Consequences for specific and diffuse forms of system justification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(3), 498-509. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.88.3.498

  • Kofta, M., Soral, W., Kwiatkowska, Z., Kapusta, S., & Mikołajczak, M. (2016). Skala preferencji tradycyjnej vs. nowoczesnej roli kobiety w społeczeństwie: Konstrukcja i walidacja [Preference of traditional vs. modern role of a woman in the society: Construction and validation]. Psychologia Spoleczna, 11(4), 474-488. https://doi.org/10.7366/1896180020163907

  • Krause, E. (2016). Macierzyństwo kobiet-naukowców: O konflikcie roli rodzicielskiej i zawodowej [Motherhood of female scientists: Parental and vocational conflict]. Szkoła-Zawód-Praca, 12, 153-167.

  • Kurowska, A. (2010). Bariery i uwarunkowania aktywności zawodowej młodych matek w Polsce [Barriers and determinants of professional activity of young mothers in Poland]. Polityka Społeczna, 2010(11–12), 11-18.

  • Łaciak, B. (2012). Ciąża i macierzyństwo. Serialowe reprezentacje jako element społecznego dyskursu [Pregnancy and motherhood: TV series representations as a part of the social discourse]. In R. E. Hryciuk & E. Korolczuk (Ed.), Pożegnanie z Matką Polką? [Farewell to the Mother Pole?] (pp. 393-420). Warsaw, Poland: WUW.

  • Malinowska, E. (1995). Socio-political changes in Poland and the problem of sex discrimination. Women’s Studies International, 18(1), 35-43. https://doi.org/10.10.16/0277-5395(94)00078-6

  • Mikołajczak, M. (2016). The structure of ambivalence toward women – Legitimization of gender discrimination and prospects for change (Doctoral thesis, University of Warsaw, Warsaw, Poland). Retrieved from https://depotuw.ceon.pl/handle/item/1367

  • Mikołajczak, M., & Pietrzak, J. (2014). Ambivalent sexism and religion: Connected through values. Sex Roles, 70(9-10), 387-399. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-014-0379-3

  • Mikołajczak, M., Pietrzak, J., & Winiewski, M. (2009). Warmth and competence of different groups of women in Poland. Unpublished data.

  • Okimoto, T. G., & Heilman, M. E. (2012). The “Bad Parent” assumption: How gender stereotypes affect reactions to working mothers. The Journal of Social Issues, 68(4), 704-724. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.2012.01772.x

  • Pietrzak, J., Mikołajczak, M., Chroł, K., & Markiewicz, M. (2011, July). The parenthood penalty/ bonus in recruitment. Poster session presented at the 34th Annual Scientific Meeting of the International Society for Political Psychology, Istanbul, Turkey.

  • Prentice, D. A., & Carranza, E. (2002). What women and men should be, shouldn’t be, are allowed to be, and don’t have to be: The contents of prescriptive gender stereotypes. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 26(4), 269-281. https://doi.org/10.1111/1471-6402.t01-1-00066

  • Rudman, L. A. (1998). Self-promotion as a risk factor for women: The costs and benefits of counter stereotypical impression management. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(3), 629-645. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.3.629

  • Rudman, L. A., & Fairchild, K. (2004). Reactions to counter stereotypic behavior: The role of backlash in cultural stereotype maintenance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(2), 157-176. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.87.2.157

  • Sigle-Rushton, W., & Waldfogel, J. (2007). Motherhood and women’s earnings in Anglo-American, continental European, and Nordic countries. Feminist Economics, 13(2), 55-91. https://doi.org/10.1080/13545700601184849

  • Szastok, M., Kossowska, M., & de Lemus, S. (2019). I am not going to take it anymore: Exposure to traditional gender relations reduces backlash against nontraditional women. Manuscript submitted for publication.

  • Titkow, A. (2001). On the appreciated role of women. In M. Ingham, H. Ingham, & H. Domański (Eds.), Women on the Polish labor market (pp. 21-40). Budapest, Hungary: Central European University Press.

  • Viki, T., & Abrams, D. (2002). But she was unfaithful: Benevolent sexism and reactions to rape victims who violate traditional gender role expectations. Sex Roles, 47(5-6), 289-293. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1021342912248