Credibility in Social and Personality Psychology

Introduction to Special Topic “Is Psychology Self-Correcting? Reflections on the Credibility Revolution in Social and Personality Psychology”

Simine Vazire*1, Brian Nosek2

Social Psychological Bulletin, 2023, Vol. 18, Article e12927, https://doi.org/10.32872/spb.12927

Published (VoR): 2023-11-17.

*Corresponding author at: Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, University of Melbourne, Parkville, VIC 3010, Australia. E-mail: simine.vazire@unimelb.edu.au

Related: This article is part of the SPB Special Topic "Is Psychology Self-Correcting? Reflections on the Credibility Revolution in Social and Personality Psychology", Guest Editors: Simine Vazire & Brian Nosek, Social Psychological Bulletin, 18, https://doi.org/10.32872/spb.v18

This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Yesterday, at a social event, one of us was chatting with another psychologist who remarked, “It is pretty incredible to be living in a period in which we get to observe the field undergo a historical transformation.” Their comment reflects a sentiment that many share—much has changed in how psychology is done and communicated during the last decade, perhaps more than in any other.

The motivation for change, and the largely grassroots activism that has advanced change, was accelerated around 2011 with events that highlighted potential weaknesses in the credibility of published evidence and claims, as well as the processes, practices, and reward systems that shape the production of evidence and claims. New approaches emerged to improve the credibility of research. Have the changes been for the better? Do the new approaches and behaviors address the right problems? What problems are being missed? What new problems are being created?

In 2021, ten years after the start of the “replication crisis” in psychology, we set out to collect a set of articles reflecting on the crisis and the ensuing “credibility revolution” or reform movement. The nine articles in this special topic provide a broad and varied perspective on this tumultuous decade.

Several of the submissions focus on ways in which the reform efforts have fallen short. These failures include, according to Beer et al. (2023, this volume): the reform movement has not been as aligned with efforts to improve diversity, equity, and inclusion in the field as it could be; it has narrowly focused on practices and researchers at research-intensive institutions; and it has focused on a narrow, homogenous subtype of research topics and designs. Another weakness of the reform movement, according to Wilson and Wixted (2023, this volume) is that most interpretations of replication projects, such as the Reproducibility Project: Psychology (Open Science Collaboration, 2015) are based on little more than intuition, while model-based approaches are more rigorous and challenge many of the intuition-based interpretations. Finally, one of the fundamental philosophical commitments (perhaps implicit) in the reform movement, the conceptualization of expertise, is misguided, according to Uygun Tunç and Tunç (2023, this volume). This has hampered progress, particularly in aligning incentives with the reform movement’s goals.

Other contributions focus on how we need to expand the scope of our self-reflection and reform efforts. In the early years of the crisis and revolution, the focus of the field’s attention was relatively narrow (mostly on p-hacking, false positives, and replication). However, the problems extend much farther. One source of error in our published research is problematic interpretive practices, and specifically the use of strategic ambiguity, according to Frankenhuis et al. (2023, this volume). Like many questionable research practices, these misleading interpretations can happen even when scientists act in good faith, and typically lead to overclaiming findings and their implications. Another overlooked source of potential errors and bias is the data from student projects, according to Ludwig et al. (2023, this volume). Their findings suggest that our pedagogical practices may encourage questionable and unethical research practices in student projects, which can easily make their way into published research. Finally, the perspective of participants is important to consider when evaluating potential threats to the validity of our research, according to Mason et al. (2023, this volume). Using qualitative evidence (from cognitive interviews and think-aloud protocols) they shed light on obstacles to straightforward interpretations of self-report data on common questionnaires.

A number of articles in this volume reflect on whether the reforms in our field have helped, and where they have fallen short. In the domain of first impressions research, Satchell et al. (2023, this volume) find that while much has improved (e.g., transparency, collaborative science, and norms around replication), there are still weak spots. In particular, the authors argue that most paradigms in this field are too asocial, and fail to reflect the rich, complex context of everyday life. Another area where we might expect improvement is in journal policies regarding replication research. Looking at social psychology journals’ policies with respect to replications, Torka et al. (2023, this volume) find that while the proportion of journal websites that explicitly welcome replication submissions has increased from 12% to 25% from 2015 to 2022, most journals still do not mention replication research on their websites. An important question for the reform movement to consider is whether the crisis, and the ensuing credibility revolution, have impacted public trust in science. Methner et al. (2023, this volume) found that lay people reported more trust in researchers if they learned about both the crisis and the reform movement, than if they learned about only the crisis (though the effect was quite small). This suggests that efforts to self-correct, when visible to the public, may help buffer any negative impact of visible failures and errors, but this may require sustained and comprehensive reforms (consistent with Schiavone & Vazire, 2023).

Although we are surely biased, as the editors who handled and accepted these articles for this special topic, we feel that the submissions themselves reflect the spirit of the revolution. They are reflective and critical, the empirical research is conducted rigorously and reported transparently, and the articles, we believe, make calibrated claims, aiming for accuracy over hype. Of course, there are just a handful of perspectives on the last decade in our field. However, they are emblematic of what may be the most important thing to emerge in the reform movement—an active and diverse research program on metascience.

The metascience movement draws on the long history of fields that examine the scientific process, and it adds a directional motive—how can we do better—and an entrepreneurial orientation—let’s try things to get better. The combination of an activist reform movement that is making change and a metascientific community that is offering solutions and evaluating their effectiveness is helping to overcome the inertia of the status quo, and the pull of system justification. When a dysfunctional system seems unchangeable, one way to resolve the dissonance of being a willing participant in it is to justify the existing system as the way it must be. Metascientists and reformers are demonstrating that the system is of our own making, and thereby subject to remaking.

The metascientific perspectives in this special topic capture both points of pride as well as mistakes or missed opportunities, and provide some fodder for reflection. Moreover, as the reform movement has spread across scholarly disciplines, the experience of psychologists and their expertise in human behavior are tremendous assets to help continue on the path towards self-improvement and increasing credibility, not just for psychology but for all domains of scholarly inquiry.

Funding

The authors have no funding to report.

Acknowledgments

The authors have no additional (i.e., non-financial) support to report.

Competing Interests

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

References

  • Beer, J., Eastwick, P., & Goh, J. G. (2023). Hits and misses in the last decade of open science: Researchers from different subfields and career stages offer personal reflections and suggestions. Social Psychological Bulletin, 18, Article e9681. https://doi.org/10.32872/spb.9681

  • Frankenhuis, W. E., Panchanathan, K., & Smaldino, P. E. (2023). Strategic ambiguity in the social sciences. Social Psychological Bulletin, 18, Article e9923. https://doi.org/10.32872/spb.9923

  • Ludwig, T., Altenmüller, M. S., Schramm, L. F. F., & Twardawski, M. (2023). Evading open science: The black box of student data collection. Social Psychological Bulletin, 18, Article e9411. https://doi.org/10.32872/spb.9411

  • Mason, J., Pownall, M., Palmer, A., & Azevedo, F. (2023). Investigating lay perceptions of psychological measures: A registered report. Social Psychological Bulletin, 18, Article e9383. https://doi.org/10.32872/spb.9383

  • Methner, N., Dahme, B., & Menzel, C. (2023). The “replication crisis” and trust in psychological science: How reforms shape public trust in psychology. Social Psychological Bulletin, 18, Article e9665. https://doi.org/10.32872/spb.9665

  • Open Science Collaboration. (2015). Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science, 349(6251), Article aac4716. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aac4716

  • Satchell, L., Jaeger, B., Jones, A., López, B., & Child, C. (2023). Beyond reliability in first impressions research: Considering validity and the need to “mix it up with folks”. Social Psychological Bulletin, 18, Article e10211. https://doi.org/10.32872/spb.10211

  • Schiavone, S. R., & Vazire, S. (2023). Reckoning with our crisis: An agenda for the field of social and personality psychology. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 18(3), 710-722. https://doi.org/10.1177/17456916221101060

  • Torka, A. K., Mazei, J., & Hüffmeier, J. (2023). Are replications mainstream now? A comparison of support for replications expressed in the policies of social psychology journals in 2015 and 2022. Social Psychological Bulletin, 18, Article e9695. https://doi.org/10.32872/spb.9695

  • Uygun Tunç, D., & Tunç, M. N. (2023). Psychology’s reform movement needs a reconceptualization of scientific expertise. Social Psychological Bulletin, 18, Article e10303. https://doi.org/10.32872/spb.10303

  • Wilson, B. M., & Wixted, J. T. (2023). On the importance of modeling the invisible world of underlying effect sizes. Social Psychological Bulletin, 18, Article e9981. https://doi.org/10.32872/spb.9981